Skip to main content

Does the expensive brain hypothesis apply to amphibians and reptiles?

Abstract

Vertebrate brains show extensive variation in relative size. The expensive brain hypothesis argues that one important source of this variation is linked to a species’ ability to generate the energy required to sustain the brain, especially during periods of unavoidable food scarcity. Here we ask whether this hypothesis, tested so far in endothermic vertebrates, also applies to ectotherms, where ambient temperature is an additional major aspect of energy balance. Phylogenetic comparative analyses of reptiles and amphibians support the hypothesis. First, relative brain size increases with higher body temperature in those species active during the day that can gain free energy by basking. Second, relative brain size is smaller among nocturnal species, which generally face less favorable energy budgets, especially when maintaining high body temperature. However, we do not find an effect of seasonal variation in ambient temperature or food on brain size, unlike in endotherms. We conclude that the factors affecting energy balance in ectotherms and endotherms are overlapping but not identical. We therefore discuss the idea that when body temperatures are seasonally very low, cognitive benefits may be thwarted and selection on larger brain size may be rare. Indeed, mammalian hibernators may show similarities to ectotherms.

Peer Review reports

Introduction

Brains have unusually high energy costs per unit tissue [1, 2], linked to the high energetic demands of synaptic transmission [3]. Because larger brains provide numerous adaptive sensorimotor and cognitive benefits in a wide range of conditions, the expensive brain hypothesis [4, 5] proposes that these high costs constrain relative brain size, despite potential fitness advantages due to the cognitive benefits of having larger brains. Specifically, it predicts that smaller brains may reflect the combined effect of periods of unavoidable reductions in net energy intake [4, 5] and competition between energy allocation to the brain and other expensive functions [6], most prominently growth and reproduction [7]. These predictions received broad empirical support among endothermic taxa. First, brain size shows well-documented tradeoffs with growth and reproduction in many lineages [5, 8]. Second, brain size is reduced where endothermic animals experience reduced energy intake during seasonal scarcity [9] or periods of unavoidable starvation [10], as on small islands. Third, the high costs of brains also suggest that offspring of larger-brained species are incapable of fulfilling the brain’s energy demands on their own, and thus need parental provisioning to support the growth and differentiation of their brains [11]. Hence, the ability to provision the young may have become a constraint on brain size evolution. This developmental version of the hypothesis is supported by strong correlated evolution between the amount of parental provisioning and brain size among birds [12]. It also explains the finding that larger-brained species have larger neonates in mammals [5].

The above studies were predominantly conducted on endothermic species. The tradeoffs between brain size and growth rate (and thus age at maturity) and reproductive output reported for endotherms are also apparent in ectotherms (anurans: [13, 14]; fishes: [15]; reptiles: [16]). Likewise, as in mammals [5] and birds [12, 13], larger-brained species tend to produce larger offsprings in lineages without post-hatching provisioning (frogs: [17]; lizards [Song et al., unpublished]; chondrichthyan fishes: [18]). However, virtually no studies have assessed whether the predicted negative effects of periodic or episodic food scarcity on brain size are also found in ectotherms.

The aim of this paper is to contribute to filling this gap. Effects of periodic scarcity are expected given that ectotherms, unlike endotherms, usually are incapable of sustained activity [19], which should make them more vulnerable to fluctuations in net food intake and the resulting periods of negative energy balance, which select for smaller brain size. The only existing study [20] found that seasonality in food was linked to reduced brain size in frogs, but their seasonality index could not distinguish between the effects of low food and of low ambient temperature. Here we develop predictions for comparative tests with ectothermic tetrapods (amphibians and reptiles), given that endothermy evolved on land (e.g. [21]) and land-based vertebrates can become at least partly independent from ambient temperatures relative to fully aquatic ectotherms (fishes) by basking, changing posture or moving between temporarily favorable microhabitats. We will test three predictions that are unique to ectotherms.

Endotherms can generate heat internally and use this to maintain high and especially stable body temperature (homeothermy). In contrast, ectotherms primarily rely on external sources of heat to regulate their body temperature, directly or through basking (especially found in reptiles: [22], and occasionally in anurans: [23]), which does not require metabolically generated energy. As a result, their energy balance is not solely dependent on food availability and intake but is also influenced by body temperature. The first two predictions therefore relate to the effects of body temperature on brain size (cf. [24, 25]).

The physiological processes of endotherms run at a higher and more stable body temperature than in the great majority of ectotherms [26], with only lizards showing partial overlap with mammals [27]. A higher body temperature has the advantage that biochemical processes run reliably fast. All physiological processes follow the Q10 rule, which means that for every 10° C rise in temperature biochemical processes run ca. 2–3 times faster [28]. Many ectotherms have evolved adaptations to remain active at far lower body temperatures than endotherms can (e.g., [23]). However, remaining active at such low body temperatures must in itself be physiologically costly. The Q10 rule indicates that we should expect that major reductions in temperature slow down nerve conduction speed and thus may have detrimental effects on higher processing in the brain of ectotherms. Thus, avoiding these negative effects requires various energetically costly structural (e.g. adding mitochondria) or biochemical adaptations (e.g. having a variety of enzymes allowing performance at a broad range of temperatures) [26]; cf. [29, 30]. This explains why different species are able to maximize physiological functions at highly different temperatures [28]. Ectotherms with high body temperature (Tb) during activity therefore can approach the optimum temperature range, but the lower mean Tb gets, the higher the physiological price of remaining active. Thus, ectotherms that are active at lower Tb should be energetically more constrained than those active at higher Tb, and this should leave less energy to be allocated to the brain. We therefore predict that higher Tb will be accompanied by increased relative brain size (Prediction 1).

Basking allows species to have Tb well above ambient temperatures during their active period. However, strictly nocturnal species cannot bask, and therefore can only maintain higher Tb relative to ambient temperatures by increasing activity levels. However, doing this requires energy and would therefore negatively affect the energy balance. As a result, we expect smaller relative brain size in nocturnal species (Prediction 2).

Unlike endotherms, ectotherms may generally be unable to generate the high and stable energy food intake needed to sustain larger brains during times of food scarcity. We expect this effect to be even stronger in ectotherms due to their reduced activity levels and mobility. Thus, we predict that seasonal food scarcity will be linked to smaller brain sizes (Prediction 3). This seemingly obvious prediction is somewhat speculative, however. Among endotherms, energy requirements remain approximately constant during periods of scarcity, or even increase when food scarcity is accompanied by lower ambient temperatures (Ta) (e.g. [31, 32]). In ectotherms, in contrast, energy needs may be reduced during periods of scarcity. They tend to becomes less active (or even enter brumation) and have reduced metabolic rates during periods of low temperature (cf. [33,34,35]) or even merely during food scarcity [36]. Thus, food intake and energy requirements may vary approximately in parallel and no energy scarcity for the brain ensues, and no brain size reduction might evolve whenever seasonal food scarcity is accompanied by colder temperatures.

We tested these three predictions in a comparative study that leveraged published data on amphibians and reptiles.

Methods

Materials and methods

Morphological data

We conducted an extensive literature search to gather the data for this study. We used the ISI Web of Science to search for all articles published before November 2022 using the search terms ‘reptile’, ‘rhynchocephalia’, ‘lizard’, ‘snake’, ‘squamata’, ‘amphibia’, ‘anura’, ‘frog’, ‘caudata’, ‘salamander’ and ‘gymnophiona’, combined with ‘brain size’, ‘brain mass’, ‘brain weight’, ‘encephalization’ and ‘brain-to-body ratio’. We excluded the terms ‘dinosaur’, ‘fossil’ and ‘extinct’. In total, we found 53 studies for reptiles and 63 studies for amphibians, which we checked for completeness using Google Scholar using the same search terms, and checking for studies that cited studies we already had. We took the mean values of brain mass (g) and body mass (g) from the same specimens (of either sex) of all species, if needed by averaging values from different studies, while excluding duplicate reports. In total, we collected brain mass and body mass of 160 lizard species, 28 snake species, the only extant rhynchocephalian, one worm lizard, 123 frog species and 54 salamander species. We used several major sources [16, 17, 37], but also always checked the original sources. The full data set is available in the Supplementary Materials.

Activity patterns

We collected data on activity period of the species with brain size data, and distinguished between a lack of basking opportunities (which pits nocturnal species against both diurnal and cathemeral ones, which were therefore combined into one category). For 358 of the 367 species, we were able to categorize their activity periods. We used several major sources [38,39,40], but also always checked the original sources. The full data set is available in the Supplementary Materials.

Ecological data: ambient temperature and food availability

Data of the geographic ranges for reptiles were obtained from recently published data source [41] and for amphibians from the IUCN [42]. We use the mean annual temperature (BIO1), as defined by [43] (data downloaded from WorldClim website: www.worldclim.org/data/bioclim) to characterize mean ambient temperature (Ta), and BIO4 as the measure of temperature seasonality. We calculated the temperature measures for the whole distribution range for each species by taking the average of all 5 × 5 km cells in their range.

To characterize seasonality in food abundance, we relied on vegetation measures linked to plant growth, because the abundance of both young foliage and insects, the main food of replies and amphibians, peaks at periods of high plant productivity [41]. Thus, we used the coefficient of variation in monthly average values of the NDVI index (normalized difference vegetation index), which estimates the abundance of chlorophyll based on Moderate Resolution Imaging Spectroradiometer (MODIS) data (MOD13C2v006). These measures were calculated from the entire geographic range (using 0.05 deg CMG grid cells, which are 5.6 × 5.6 km) for 20 years from 2001 to 2020. Because detailed geographic ranges were missing for a few species, we had mean temperature and NDVI data for 363 and 361 species, respectively.

Body temperature

Records of Tb were collected from published sources (as detailed in the Data accessibility section). We only included mean Tb records during the animals’ active period, and therefore excluded both data from animals that were brumating and records of preferred Tb in lab studies. For species with multiple reports, we took the average of the mean Tb for each population. In total, we collected mean body temperature for 48 amphibian and 105 reptile species. The major source is [40], but we also always checked the original sources. The full data set is available in the Supplementary Materials. We found too few reliable data on temporal variation in Tb of active animals to be able to include this measure in our analyses.

Statistical analyses

We implemented all statistical analyses in R 4.1.1 [44], using the package phylolm [45] to control for any effects of phylogenetic non-independence. The figures were generated in the package ggplot2 [46]. To visualize the results, the figures show residual brain size, even though the analyses were based on absolute brain size while controlling for body mass as a covariate.

To test the predicted correlated evolution between relative brain size and Tb (P1) and nocturnality (P2), we fitted a phylogenetic generalized least squares regression (PGLS) with absolute brain mass (log 10 transferred) as the dependent variable and body mass (log 10 transferred), taxon (reptile or amphibian), activity period (diurnal and cathemeral species have opportunities for basking or use the higher ambient temperatures accompanying daytime, and were therefore combined, whereas nocturnal ones do not), mean Tb, as well as the interaction between activity period and mean Tb. We first tested the overall fit of the model by comparing the full model, including the predictors (activity period and Tb), and control variables (body mass and taxon) with the null model (which only includes the control variables) using a likelihood ratio test [47]. Subsequently, we extended the likelihood ratio test (LRT) by comparing the full model with a model that also included an interaction between activity period and mean Tb. No multicollinearity was observed among the four independent variables (Table S1). Visual inspection of all model fits confirmed that they satisfied model assumptions (including normally distributed model residuals, and homogeneity of the variance [48]). Additionally, where the interaction between body temperature and activity period showed a significant effect on brain, we separated the PGLS model by activity period in order to fully investigate the predictions 1 & 2, i.e., whether Tb had a positive or negative effect on brain size depending on activity period. To control for phylogenetic uncertainty in tree reconstruction, we used a recently published time-calibrated multi-tree phylogeny for reptiles [49] and amphibians [50], with randomly selected 100 trees to run PGLS models for each lineage.

To test the predicted correlated evolution between relative brain size and seasonality in food (P3), we fitted PGLS models with absolute brain mass (log 10 transferred) as the dependent variable and body mass (log 10 transferred), taxon (reptile or amphibian), as well as the activity period, mean and seasonality in ambient temperature, and the mean and CV (coefficient of variation) of NDVI per year as independent variables (the latter all untransformed). Because of multicollinearity among the climate and NDVI variables (see Table S1), we did several independent analyses with one of the collinear variables removed. As above, we visually assessed whether model assumptions were satisfied. This same analysis was repeated for a subset of species for which we had mean body temperatures, which was also included as an independent variable (see Table S3).

Results

We first examined the effect of Tb (mean body temperature) on brain mass. Including activity period and Tb into the model significantly improved model fit, as evidenced by a significant LRT (X2 = 6.649, p = 0.036). Furthermore, the interaction between activity period and Tb also yielded a significant enhancement to the model compared to the one without this interaction term (LRT, X2 = 11.626, p < 0.001). As documented in Table 1, there was a significant effect of activity period, but its effect interacted with that of Tb: in diurnal or cathemeral species, which have opportunities to warm their bodies through basking, relative brain size increased with Tb, whereas in nocturnal species it decreased (Fig. 1). Separate analyses for these two categories of activity periods showed that both effects were significant (Table S2). For diurnal and cathemeral species, this result confirms the correlated evolution between relative brain size and Tb (Prediction 1). In contrast, nocturnal species had smaller relative brain sizes, but the interaction effect revealed that this only held for species with higher Tb (Fig. 1), which confirms the predicted negative correlation between nocturnality and relative brain size (Prediction 2).

Table 1 PGLS analyses of the effects of body temperature and activity period on brain size, while controlling for various confounding effects. Variables with significant effects (P < 0.05) are highlighted in in bold. The 95% confidence intervals for the 100 trees are shown in brackets. λ is Pagel’s lambda
Fig. 1
figure 1

Relative brain size increases with mean body temperature in diurnal & cathemeral species, while decreases in nocturnal species. Lines were depict the PGLS equations

We found no correlated evolution between relative brain size and seasonality in food (disconfirming Prediction 3). While controlling for the effects of body size, taxon (reptiles versus amphibians), and activity period, no effect was found for either mean ambient temperature or the mean or CV in the NDVI (Table 2) nor of seasonality of ambient temperature (Table S3) in the full sample (n = 350 species). When we repeated this analysis for the subset of species (n = 148) for which we had information on mean Tb, the results remained the same (Tables S4 & S5).

Table 2 PGLS analyses of the effects of the ambient environment including average ambient temperature, average and CV of NDVI on brain size, while controlling for various confounding effects. Variables with significant effects (P < 0.05) are highlighted in in bold. The 95% confidence intervals for the 100 trees are shown in brackets. λ is Pagel’s lambda

As elaborated in the supplementary analyses, these results were robust against examining the role of possible confounding variables (other aspects of the ecological niche or major differences in body plan) and using alternative ways of calculating existing variables (using the centroid rather than the mean of the geographic range).

Discussion

In this study, we tested three predictions of the expensive brain hypothesis on relative brain size in tetrapod ectotherms. Overall, we found good support for the effect of body temperature (P1) and activity period (P2), but found no evidence for the hypothesized effect of seasonality in food (P3).

In species potentially able to bask (i.e., diurnal and cathemeral ones), mean body temperature while active (Tb) was positively correlated with brain size. This pattern supports the idea that species able to achieve higher Tb from environmental sources have, other things being equal, a more favorable energy balance (P1). In nocturnal species, brain size decreased with Tb (P2), supporting the idea that costly biochemical adaptations or muscle activity [51] are needed to maintain a higher Tb during the night, when ambient temperatures are reduced and no basking is possible. Doing so must negatively affect energy balance, and thus brain size. In general, reptiles in our dataset live in warm habitats (Figure S1; see also [41]) and accordingly their Tb during their active period are above the mean ambient temperature (Figure S2). In both reptiles and amphibians, Tb is also higher in diurnal and cathemeral species than in nocturnal species (Figure S2), reflecting both basking opportunities and warmer ambient temperatures. Nocturnal reptiles live in warmer habitats than diurnal or cathemeral ones but this difference is not found in amphibians (Figure S1). This pattern reflects that reptiles are both more active and more likely to bask when diurnal or cathemeral, and consequently achieve favorable Tb even in colder habitats [52, 53], whereas amphibians are capable of remaining active at far lower body and ambient temperatures [23].

Overall, these findings therefore show that quadrupedal ectotherms often have smaller brains when they have higher energy costs. For nocturnal reptiles these costs may be higher because they cannot bask and remain active during periods of lower Tb, whereas for amphibians they are higher when they live in cooler habitats (and cannot bask: many amphibians rapidly dehydrate when they bask: [23]). This later finding echoes suggestive findings in fishes that noted a correlation between seawater temperatures and relative brain size, although food scarcity may be even more responsible ([54], see also [24]).

We found no effect of seasonality in food availability on brain size (P3), unlike in endotherms. Of course, the measure of seasonality in food abundance was crude (landscape-level NDVI) and we also lacked estimates of actual food intake, unlike in primate studies (e.g., [55]). Thus, the rejection of the prediction may be a false negative. Alternatively, it is likely that the periods of low food intake are accompanied by low temperatures and thus by low activity levels and metabolism, since seasonal variation in ambient temperature and in NDVI were highly correlated. However, the same thing may also happen in tropical habitats when food is scarce [33, 36]. Either way, net energy intake and energy requirements may therefore fluctuate roughly in parallel, which would explain the lack of a negative impact of seasonality in food abundance.

These results therefore suggest it is too early to conclude that seasonal food scarcity affects brain size in ectotherms in the same way as in endotherms. The study that found this, in a sample of subtropical frogs ([20]), did not control for seasonality in ambient temperatures, which might explain their result. Most endotherms remain active and are more likely to respond cognitively by shifts in diet or foraging strategy, even if their brain size is somewhat reduced. However, those endotherms that respond to food scarcity physiologically by hibernating have far smaller brains than non-hibernators [56]. This strategy, called brumation in ectotherms, is widespread among ectotherms, and may thus explain the reduced brain size of brumating species, as found in Anura [57]. However, in constrast to the main endotherm pattern, we found that ectotherms that remain active at lower Tb have smaller brains, no doubt linked to reduced metabolism. This makes them more tolerant of longer periods of starvation compared to endotherms [58], just like hibernating mammals tend to have better survival than non-hibernating ones [59]. Thus, whether it is due to food scarcity or colder temperatures, many ectotherms appear to respond to unfavorable periods by reducing metabolism, unlike in endotherms [e.g., [31, 32]].

This non-cognitive strategy of coping with food scarcity is entirely consistent with the expensive brain hypothesis. However, the expensive brain hypothesis assumes that brains are as large as the species can afford energetically, and therefore that larger brains bring fitness benefits in terms of perception, cognition, and action (review: [11]). In contrast, if these processes are much slowed down by lower Tb, such fitness benefits may not accrue. For instance, even a modest reduction in incubation temperature (from 220 to 160C) has been shown to affect subsequent learning in a scincid lizard [60], echoing earlier results on learning in fishes acclimated at lower water (and thus body) temperatures [61, 62]. In that case, larger brains would not bring cognitive benefits, and selection could only rarely favor brain size increases beyond a size that supports those functions that show the steepest marginal effects on fitness and remain possible at lower Tb.

To test this alternative explanation for the reduced brain size in species with greater seasonal variation in ambient temperature we would need more independent evidence of Tb effects on perception and cognition, in particular learning and memory, of animals active at low Tb. However, the fit with predictions from the expensive brain hypothesis for the basking taxa, which have higher Tb, especially in the more active reptiles (cf. Figure S2), suggests this alternative may especially apply to taxa with the lowest Tb and living in the coolest habitats, and thus in particular, though not exclusively, to amphibians.

This alternative explanation may also hold for hibernating endotherms. In them, the major programmed and long-term reduction in Tb and all metabolic processes are correlated with strongly reduced relative brain size [56]. The expensive brain hypothesis suggests this is due to less favorable energy balance during the long period of hibernation, when the organism lives off its accumulated body fat. The alternative interpretation is that brains lose learned skills during long cold periods, thus preventing selection in favor of larger brains. Although the empirical evidence is curiously mixed, various results indicate that the price for having this physiological adaptation against food scarcity may be that the long period of hypothermia due to hibernation reduces cognitive performance. Thus, various studies report clear negative effects of prolonged hypothermia on memory [63,64,65,66], presumably linked to the documented pronounced seasonal reductions in brain activity [67], neural connectivity [68], or even overall brain size [69]. Moreover, emergence from hibernation is accompanied by pronounced synaptic remodeling in the hypothalamus, suggesting high relearning activity [68]. Indeed, ground squirrels show far better learning performance right after emerging from hibernation than a month later [70] or than control individuals prevented from hibernating [71]. This pattern is expected if hibernation affects the retention of learned knowledge and motor skills. Such a negative effect of long-term cooling may apply even more to ectotherms, given that brumation, the passive reduction in body temperature leading to immobility, differs from mammalian hibernation by not being occasionally interrupted by arousal [72].

Although these results are partly contradictory, they suggest that in organisms forced to remain active at unusually low Tb, such as amphibians tied to water and unable to bask, like all Caudata and many Anura [23], the benefits of larger brain size do not accrue as much as to reptiles, with their higher Tb and opportunities for basking. As a result, selection would more rarely favor larger brains in organisms forced to live at low Tb, unless they live in unusually food-rich environments. We hope that future work will help to examine this alternative hypothesis.

Clearly, the comparative tests reported here cannot be the final word. First, limits on the availability of brain data forced us to exclude turtles and crocodilians among reptiles and caecilians among amphibians. Second, mean Tbs were available only for a fraction of the species. Third, in the absence of data on seasonality in food or climate of the actual habitats inhabited by particular species, we relied on regional measures of seasonality, in particular variation in NDVI (which reflects variation in plant production, and thus insect availability). Nonetheless, we feel the results were robust enough to justify more detailed follow-up studies.

Data availability

All data are available in the supplementary material.

References

  1. Mink JW, Blumenschine RJ, Adams DB. Ratio of central nervous system to body metabolism in vertebrates: its constancy and functional basis. Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol. 1981;241:R203–12.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  2. Bauernfeind AL, Barks SK, Duka T, Grossman LI, Hof PR, Sherwood CC. Aerobic glycolysis in the primate brain: reconsidering the implications for growth and maintenance. Brain Struct Funct. 2014;219:1149–67.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  3. Li S, Sheng Z-H. Energy matters: presynaptic metabolism and the maintenance of synaptic transmission. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2022;23:4–22.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  4. Allman JM. Evolving brains. New York, NY.: Scientific American Library; 1999.

    Google Scholar 

  5. Isler K, van Schaik CP. The expensive brain: a framework for explaining evolutionary changes in brain size. J Hum Evol. 2009;57:392–400.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Aiello LC, Wheeler P. The expensive-tissue hypothesis: the brain and the digestive system in human and primate evolution. Curr Anthropol. 1995;36:199–221.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Navarrete A, van Schaik CP, Isler K. Energetics and the evolution of human brain size. Nature. 2011;480:91–3.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  8. Schuppli C, Isler K, van Schaik CP. How to explain the unusually late age at skill competence among humans. J Hum Evol. 2012;63:843–50.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. van Woerden JT, Willems EP, van Schaik CP, Isler K. Large brains buffer energetic effects of seasonal habitats in catarrhine primates. Evol (N Y). 2012;66:191–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. Heldstab SA, Isler K, Graber SM, Schuppli C, van Schaik CP. The economics of brain size evolution in vertebrates. Curr Biol. 2022;32:R697–708.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  11. van Schaik CP, Song Z, Schuppli C, Drobniak SM, Heldstab SA, Griesser M. Extended parental provisioning and variation in vertebrate brain sizes. PLoS Biol. 2023;21:e3002016.

  12. Griesser M, Drobniak SM, Graber SM, van Schaik C. Parental provisioning drives brain size in birds. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2023;120. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1073/pnas.2121467120

  13. Mai CL, Liao WB. Brain size evolution in anurans: a review. Anim Biol. 2019;69:265–79.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Yu X, Zhong MJ, Li DY, Jin L, Liao WB, Kotrschal A. Large-brained frogs mature later and live longer. Evolution. 2018;72:1174–83.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Fischer S, Jungwirth A. The costs and benefits of larger brains in fishes. J Evol Biol. 2022;35:973–85.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Stark G, Pincheira-Donoso D. The evolution of brain size in ectothermic tetrapods: large brain mass trades-off with lifespan in reptiles. Evol Biol. 2022. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1007/s11692-022-09562-4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. Liao WB, Lou SL, Zeng Y, Kotrschal A. Large brains, small guts: the expensive tissue hypothesis supported within anurans. Am Nat. 2016;188:693–700.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Mull CG, Yopak KE, Dulvy NK. Does more maternal investment mean a larger brain? Evolutionary relationships between reproductive mode and brain size in chondrichthyans. Mar Freshw Res. 2011;62:567.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  19. Bennett AF, Ruben JA. Endothermy and activity in vertebrates. Science. 1979;206:649–54. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1126/science.493968

  20. Luo Y, Zhong MJ, Huang Y, Li F, Liao WB, Kotrschal A. Seasonality and brain size are negatively associated in frogs: evidence for the expensive brain framework. Sci Rep. 2017;7:1–9.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  21. Striedter GF, Northcutt RG. Brains through time: a natural history of vertebrates. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2020.

    Google Scholar 

  22. Shine R. Life-history evolution in reptiles. Annu Rev Ecol Evol Syst. 2005;36:23–46.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. Wells KD. The ecology and behavior of amphibians. Chicago. Illinois, USA.: University of Chicago Press; 2007.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  24. Yu Y, Karbowski J, Sachdev RN, Feng J. Effect of temperature and glia in brain size enlargement and origin of allometric body-brain size scaling in vertebrates. BMC Evol Biol. 2014;14:178.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  25. Gillooly JF, McCoy MW. Brain size varies with temperature in vertebrates. PeerJ. 2014;2. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.7717/peerj.301

  26. Prosser CL, Nelson DO. The role of nervous systems in temperature adaptation of poikilotherms. Annu Rev Physiol. 1981;43:281–300.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  27. Moreira MO, Qu Y, Wiens JJ. Large-scale evolution of body temperatures in land vertebrates. Evol Lett. 2021;5:484–94.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  28. McNab BK. The physiological ecology of vertebrates: a view from energetics. New York: Cornell University Press; 2002.

    Google Scholar 

  29. Somero GN. Adaptation of enzymes to temperature: searching for basic strategies. Comp Biochem Physiol B Biochem Mol Biol. 2004;139:321–33.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  30. Hochachka PW. Channels and pumps—determinants of metabolic cold adaptation strategies. Comp Biochem Physiol Part B: Comp Biochem. 1988;90:515–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  31. Sgueo C, Wells ME, Russell DE, Schaeffer PJ. Acclimatization of seasonal energetics in northern cardinals (Cardinalis cardinalis) through plasticity of metabolic rates and ceilings. J Exp Biol. 2012;215:2418–24.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Bozinovic F, Bacigalupe LD, Vásquez RA, Henk Visser G, Veloso C, Kenagy GJ. Cost of living in free-ranging degus (Octodon degus): seasonal dynamics of energy expenditure. Comp Biochem Physiol A Mol Integr Physiol. 2004;137:597–604.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  33. Berg W, Theisinger O, Dausmann KH. Acclimatization patterns in tropical reptiles: uncoupling temperature and energetics. Sci Nat. 2017;104:91.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  34. Lillywhite H. Behavior and physiology: an ecological and evolutionary viewpoint on the energy and water relations of ectothermic amphibians and reptiles. Amphibian and Reptile adaptations to the Environment. Boca Raton: CRC Press; 2017. pp. 41–62.

    Google Scholar 

  35. Angilletta Jr. MJ. Thermal adaptation. Oxford University Press; 2009.

  36. Christian KA, Bedford GS, Schultz TJ. Energetic consequences of metabolic depression in tropical and temperate-zone lizards. Aust J Zool. 1999;47:133.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  37. Amiel JJ, Tingley R, Shine R. Smart moves: effects of relative brain size on establishment success of invasive amphibians and reptiles. PLoS ONE. 2011;6:e18277.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  38. Oliveira BF, São-Pedro VA, Santos-Barrera G, Penone C, Costa GC. AmphiBIO, a global database for amphibian ecological traits. Sci Data. 2017;4:170123.

  39. Huang CH, Zhong MJ, Liao WB, Kotrschal A. Investigating the role of body size, ecology, and behavior in anuran eye size evolution. Evol Ecol. 2019;33:585–98.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  40. Meiri S. Traits of lizards of the world: variation around a successful evolutionary design. Glob Ecol Biogeogr. 2018;27:1168–72.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  41. Roll U, Feldman A, Novosolov M, Allison A, Bauer AM, Bernard R, et al. The global distribution of tetrapods reveals a need for targeted reptile conservation. Nat Ecol Evol. 2017;1:1677–82.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. IUCN (International Union for Conservation of Nature). The IUCN Red List of Threatened Species. 2021.

  43. Karger DN, Conrad O, Böhner J, Kawohl T, Kreft H, Soria-Auza RW, et al. Climatologies at high resolution for the earth’s land surface areas. Sci Data. 2017;4:1–20.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  44. R Core Team. R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R foundation for statistical computing, Vienna, Austria. 2021.

  45. Ho L si, Ane T. A linear-time algorithm for Gaussian and non-Gaussian trait evolution models. Syst Biol. 2014;63:397–408. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1093/sysbio/syu005

  46. Wickham H. ggplot2. Wiley Interdiscip Rev Comput Stat. 2011;3:180–5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  47. Fox J. Applied regression analysis and generalized linear models. Thousand Oaks, California.: Sage Publications; 2015.

    Google Scholar 

  48. Harrell FE. Regression modeling strategies: with applications to linear models, logistic and ordinal regression, and survival analysis. Cham: Springer International Publishing; 2015.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  49. Tonini JFR, Beard KH, Ferreira RB, Jetz W, Pyron RA. Fully-sampled phylogenies of squamates reveal evolutionary patterns in threat status. Biol Conserv. 2016;204:23–31.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  50. Jetz W, Pyron RA. The interplay of past diversification and evolutionary isolation with present imperilment across the amphibian tree of life. Nat Ecol Evol. 2018;2:850–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. Autumn K, Weinstein RB, Full RJ. Low cost of locomotion increases performance at low temperature in a nocturnal lizard. Physiol Zool. 1994;67:238–62.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  52. Angilletta MJ Jr. Thermal adaptation: a theoretical and empirical synthesis. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press; 2009.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  53. Muñoz MM, Stimola MA, Algar AC, Conover A, Rodriguez AJ, Landestoy MA, et al. Evolutionary stasis and lability in thermal physiology in a group of tropical lizards. Proc Royal Soc B: Biol Sci. 2014;281:20132433.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  54. Iglesias TL, Dornburg A, Brandley MC, Alfaro ME, Warren DL. Life in the unthinking depths: energetic constraints on encephalization in marine fishes. J Evol Biol. 2015;28:1080–90.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  55. van Woerden JT, van Schaik CP, Isler K. Brief communication: seasonality of diet composition is related to brain size in new world monkeys. Am J Phys Anthropol. 2014;154:628–32.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  56. Heldstab SA, Isler K, van Schaik CP. Hibernation constrains brain size evolution in mammals. J Evol Biol. 2018;31:1582–8.

  57. Liao WB, Jiang Y, Jin L, Lüpold S. How hibernation in frogs drives brain and reproductive evolution in opposite directions. Elife. 2023;12. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.7554/eLife.88236.1

  58. McCue MD. Starvation physiology: reviewing the different strategies animals use to survive a common challenge. Comp Biochem Physiol A Mol Integr Physiol. 2010;156:1–18.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  59. Turbill C, Bieber C, Ruf T. Hibernation is associated with increased survival and the evolution of slow life histories among mammals. Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2011;278:3355–63.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  60. Amiel JJ, Shine R. Hotter nests produce smarter young lizards. Biol Lett. 2012;8:372–4.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  61. Rahmann H, Schmidt W, Schmidt B. Influence of long-term thermal acclimation on the conditionability of fish. J Therm Biol. 1980;5:11–6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  62. Brezden BL, Fenwick JC, Moon TW. The effects of acclimation temperature and conditioning temperature on the learning rate of the goldfish carassius auratus. Comp Biochem Physiol A Physiol. 1975;50:373–7.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  63. Mateo JM, Johnston RE. Retention of social recognition after hibernation in Belding’s ground squirrels. Anim Behav. 2000;59:491–9.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  64. Hernández-Montero JR, Schöner CR, Kerth G. No evidence for memory retention of a learned association between a cue and roost quality after hibernation in free‐ranging bats. Ethology. 2020;126:761–71.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  65. Millesi E, Prossinger H, Dittami JP, Fieder M. Hibernation effects on memory in European ground squirrels (Spermophilus citellus). J Biol Rhythms. 2001;16:264–71.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  66. Mactutus CF, Ferek JM, George CA, Riccio DC. Hypothermia-induced amnesia for newly acquired and old reactivated memories: commonalities and distinctions. Physiological Psychol. 1982;10:79–95.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  67. Daan S, Barnes BM, Strijkstra AM. Warming up for sleep? — ground squirrels sleep during arousals from hibernation. Neurosci Lett. 1991;128:265–8.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  68. Popov VI, Bocharova LS, Bragin AG. Repeated changes of dendritic morphology in the hippocampus of ground squirrels in the course of hibernation. Neuroscience. 1992;48:45–51.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  69. Lázaro J, Hertel M, Sherwood CC, Muturi M, Dechmann DKN. Profound seasonal changes in brain size and architecture in the common shrew. Brain Struct Funct. 2018;223:2823–40.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  70. Weltzin MM, Zhao HW, Drew KL, Bucci DJ. Arousal from hibernation alters contextual learning and memory. Behav Brain Res. 2006;167:128–33.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  71. Mihailović LJ, Petrović-Minić B, Protić S, Divac I. Effects of hibernation on learning and retention. Nature. 1968;218:191–2.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  72. Geiser F, Hibernation. Curr Biol. 2013;23:R188–93.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

Download references

Funding

MG was supported by a Heisenberg Grant GR 4650/2 − 1 by the German Research Foundation DFG.

Open Access funding enabled and organized by Projekt DEAL.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

ZS and CPvS designed research; ZS aggregated, analyzed and interpreted data; ZS, CS, CPvS and MG wrote the manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Zitan Song.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

The present research study focused solely on the collection and analysis of publicly available data; therefore, no specific ethical approval or consent was required.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Electronic supplementary material

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

Supplementary Material 1:

Supplementary materials

Supplementary Material 2:

Dataset

Supplementary Material 3:

R code

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Song, Z., Griesser, M., Schuppli, C. et al. Does the expensive brain hypothesis apply to amphibians and reptiles?. BMC Ecol Evo 23, 77 (2023). https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/s12862-023-02188-w

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/s12862-023-02188-w

Keywords