Skip to main content
  • Research article
  • Open access
  • Published:

Birth size and morphological femininity in adult women

Abstract

Background

Women’s morphological femininity is perceived to develop under the influence of sex hormones and to serve as a cue of estradiol level, fertility and health in mating context. However, as the studies on direct relationship between femininity and sex steroid levels have reported mixed results, it is still not well understood what factors contribute to inter-women variation in morphological femininity. Epidemiological studies show that indicators of adverse conditions during intrauterine growth and development in utero, such as low birthweight or relative thinness at birth, influence women’s physiology ovarian functioning and may be associated with life-time exposure to estradiol in women. Thus, here we tested if birth parameters are also related with the level of morphological femininity in adult women.

Results

One hundred sixty-five healthy women of mean age 28.47 years (SD = 2.39) participated in the study. Facial femininity was estimated based on facial width-to-height ratio (fWHR) and facial shape sexual dimorphism measured in the photos. Body femininity was estimated based on waist-to-hip ratio (WHR) and breast size. Birth weight and birth length were obtained from medical records and ponderal index at birth was calculated. No relationship between birth parameters and facial or body femininity in women of reproductive age was found, also when controlled for adult sex steroid levels and BMI.

Conclusions

The results suggest that, although previous research showed that birth parameters predict reproductive development and adult oestradiol level, they do not explain the variance in morphological femininity in women of reproductive age, trait that is thought to be a cue of a woman’s estradiol level and fertility in mating context.

Background

Sexual dimorphism, one of the most studied morphological traits as a putative cue to heritable fitness benefits [14, 17, 49, 65, 74], develops at puberty, under the influence of sex hormones. In women, estradiol promotes development of body and facial feminine traits, such as increased body fat around hips, buttocks, thighs, and breast, higher cheekbones, rounder forehead and eyes, smaller chin and nose [29, 45, 63]. As estradiol is a key fertility hormone [47], exerts immunostimulating [78], anti-inflammatory [58], cardio-protective [5], and anti-oxidant properties [71], morphological femininity is expected to signal a woman’s fertility and health. Furthermore, feminine traits are perceived as attractive [49, 55, 66], what supports their role as a biological cue.

However, although inter-individual differences in the level of morphological femininity have been argued to be linked with variation in reproductive hormone levels [55], research on the relationship between sex steroids and femininity has reported mixed results. Some studies have reported that estradiol level is positively correlated with body [34] and facial femininity [14, 45], whereas other studies have failed to confirm this relationship [62]. Also, in some studies, femininity has been shown to correlate positively with long-term health [21, 74], what might be mediated by health protective properties of estradiol, whereas in others no such relationship (both with actual health and health during puberty) has been found [65].

One explanation for these equivocal results may be that morphological femininity is rather related with life-time exposure to estradiol levels, but not necessarily with adult estradiol level, that may be altered in response to changes in BMI [51], diet [19, 37], level of physical activity and energy expenditure [30, 31]. As such, one may expect that morphological femininity level will be related with traits underlying life-time exposure to estradiol level. Human epidemiological studies, supported by animal models, show that conditions during intrauterine growth are one of the key factors shaping adult physiology [8, 11, 20, 44, 50]. Indices of foetal development, such as size at birth, have been shown to affect ovarian development [13, 27] and life-time ovarian functioning [32], as well as age at menarche and at menopause [1]. Furthermore, birth size has been also shown to be positively related with estradiol level in adulthood [18, 32, 33]. Birth weight was also found to be positively related with breast cancer risk in women ([2, 15, 26, 52]; however see for contradictory results: [70]). The suggested mechanism for this is life-time exposure to high levels of estradiol [60].

Thus, the aim of this study was to test if birth size contributes to the variation in women’s facial and body femininity in reproductive age. As intra-uterine development impacts adult physiology and may underlie life-time exposure to estradiol level, we hypothesized, that birth weight and adiposity (evaluated by ponderal index) are positively related with facial and body femininity in women of reproductive age. As adult body adiposity [22] and sex steroid levels [14, 34, 45] have been shown to contribute to the level of a woman’s morphological femininity, and also previous studies suggests that the level of morphological femininity may fluctuate following short time changes in estradiol level [45, 40, 54]; although see also [48] for negative results) we controlled for these variables in the analyses.

Results

Descriptive statistics

Mean values, standard deviations, and ranges of birth parameters, femininity measurements and sex hormones levels are presented in Table 1. fWHR correlated positively with facial shape sexual dimorphism (facial SD) (r = 0.38, p < 0.001; 95%CI [0.24;0.50]) and with body WHR (r = 0.27, p < 0.001; 95%CI [0.12;0.40]), but not with breast size (r = 0.02, p = 0.78; 95%CI [− 0.13;0.17]). Similarly, facial SD was positively related with body WHR (r = 0.22, p = 0.04; 95%CI [0.07;0.36]), but not with breast size (r = 0.06, p = 0.47; 95%CI [− 0.09;0.21]. Furthermore, body WHR was not related with breast size (r = 0.03, p = 0.70; 95%CI [− 0.12;0.18].

Table 1 Descriptive statistics (N = 165)

The relationship between birth parameters and femininity level

The results of zero-order correlations between birth parameters and femininity measurements are presented in Table 2. Additionally, we found no correlation between birth weight and tT (r = 0.13, p = 0.08; 95%CI [− 0.02;0.28]) or E2 (r = − 0.03, p = 0.69; 95%CI [− 0.18;0.12]). We also found no relationship between birth ponderal index and tT (r = 0.04, p = 0.57; 95%CI [− 0.11;0.19]) or E2 (r = 0.02, p = 0.82; 95%CI [− 0.13;0.17]). None of the facial or body femininity measurements was related with testosterone or estradiol levels (in each case p > 0.05). Furthermore, BMI was not related with birth weight (r = 0.07, p = 0.38; 95%CI [− 0.08;0.22]) or ponderal index (r = 0.07, p = 0.37; 95%CI [− 0.08;0.22]) but was positively related with fWHR (r = 0.31, p < 0.001; 95%CI [0.16;0.44]), facial SD (r = 0.27, p < 0.001; 95%CI [0.12;0.40]), WHR (r = 0.45, p < 0.001; 95%CI [0.32;0.56]), and breast size (r = 0.15, p = 0.049; 95%CI [− 0.003;0.29]).

Table 2 The results of zero-order correlations between birth parameters and femininity measurements (N = 165)

The results of multiple regression analyses showed no relationship between birth weight and facial or body femininity, also when controlled for pregnancy week at birth, BMI and sex steroids levels (Table 3). The confirmed that fWHR (Table 3 – Model 1), facial SD (Table 3 - Model 2), and body WHR (Table 3 – Model 2) were related only with women’s BMI, but not with birth weight or sex steroids levels. Breast size was not related neither with birth weight, nor with BMI or sex steroid levels (Table 3 – Model 4).

Table 3 The results of regression analyses for the relationship between birth weight and the femininity measurements, controlled for pregnancy week at birth, BMI, and sex steroids levels. Bolded results are statistically significant (N = 166)

Similar results were obtained for the relationship between ponderal index and measures of facial and body femininity, also when controlled for pregnancy week at birth, BMI and sex steroids levels (Table 4). The results showed that fWHR (Table 4 – Model 1), facial SD (Table 4 - Model 2) and body WHR (Table 4 – Model 2) were positively related with women’s BMI, but not with ponderal index at birth or sex steroids levels. Breast size was not related neither with birth ponderal index, nor with BMI or sex steroid levels (Table 4 – Model 4).

Table 4 The results of regression analyses for the relationship between birth ponderal index and femininity measurements, controlled for pregnancy week at birth, BMI, and sex steroids levels. Bolded results are statistically significant (N = 165)

Comparison of women from the lowest and the highest tercile of birth weight showed no difference in fWHR (t (105) = 0.67, p = 0.51), face sexual dimorphism (t (105) = − 0.30, p = 0.76), body WHR (t (105) = 0.22, p = 0.82) or breast size (t (105) = 0.43, p = 0.66). Also, comparison of women from the lowest and the highest tercile of birth ponderal index showed no difference in fWHR (t (91) = − 0.69, p = 0.49), face sexual dimorphism (t (108) = − 0.66, p = 0.51), body WHR (t (108) = − 0.23, p = 0.82) or breast size (t (108) = − 0.84, p = 0.40).

Conclusions

The results of our study showed that birth parameters were not related with morphological femininity in women of reproductive age. The results were similar when controlled for pregnancy week at birth, adult BMI and sex steroids levels. Thus, although previous research showed, that birth parameters predict women’s reproductive physiology [12, 13], ovarian development, and life-time exposure to estradiol level [18, 27, 28, 32, 33], the results of our study suggest that intrauterine development does not contribute to the variance in morphological femininity in women of reproductive age.

The results of our study are in line with the studies that failed to show the relationship between birth size and adult sex hormone levels [53, 57, 75]. Jasieńska et al. [32] showed a positive correlation between birth size and oestradiol level in women of reproductive age, based on repeated E2 measurements during menstrual cycle (in contrast to [75], who used only a single measurement of E2), but their study sample included individuals with very low birth weight (< 1500 g), what might result from genetic or developmental disorders that also impact postnatal development, and might have confounded the results. Furthermore, Jasieńska et al. [32] showed that the relationship between birth weight and E2 was in fact observed only in women with lower birth weight (< 3000 g), suggesting a non-linear relationship between birth size and ovarian functioning in adulthood. In our study we found no difference in terms of morphological femininity between women with birth weight or ponderal index from the lowest and the highest tercile.

Furthermore, the results of this study are in line with findings suggesting no or weak relationship between facial sexual dimorphism and sex hormone levels or health [6, 65], questioning popular and influential hypothesis that sexually dimorphic traits are valid cues of an individual’s biological condition [67]. Although facial femininity, breast size, and WHR, are expected to reflect sex steroids levels, and as such fertility in women [34, 35, 45, 59], research show that these associations are weak, and have been inconsistent across the few studies directly examining this relationship in women [4, 16, 24, 36, 42]. Here, we also found no relationship between morphological facial and body femininity and reproductive hormone levels, suggesting that femininity is not a cue of a woman’s fertility.

The only variable related with facial and body femininity in our study was BMI. Similar relationship was shown in a number of studies on facial sexual dimorphism, showing that BMI is positively associated with fWHR in adults [23, 43, 46]. Previous research have showed that men are perceived as more masculine if they appear taller and heavier, independent of how much their facial shape differs from women’s [25]. Although, so far, there have been no similar studies conducted for femininity in women, one may presume that morphological sexual dimorphism may develop in relation with height and adiposity. This may happen under the influence of somatotropic hormones like growth hormone [73] or factors secreted by adipose tissue, what might obscure the relationship between sexual dimorphism and birth parameters.

Lack of the relationship between birth parameters and morphological femininity in women of reproductive age may also result from the fact that not all children with intrauterine growth retardation (IUGR) are characterised by low birth weight (LBW). Some children are smaller at birth due to their genetic predispositions, not unfavourable intrauterine environment, and these children may not be at higher risk of developmental disturbances resulting in lower health in adulthood. Similarly, some children with IUGR may not be characterised with low birth weight, due to their genetic predisposition for greater height [64]. Furthermore, maternal undernutrition in late gestation may influence the offspring’s body proportion without affecting birth weight [3]. As such, birth weight or ponderal index may not be sufficient to identify children with IUGR [61]. Additionally, most children with IUGR in western societies undergo catch-up growth during infancy [10, 76], and children who experience accelerated growth in early postnatal life are in greater risk of developing health problems related with IUGR [7, 9, 56]. Research suggests that this rapid postnatal catch-up growth of LBW neonates is even more important for adult health than birth weight alone [39]. Thus, it may be possible that the relationship between birth parameters and morphological femininity in women of reproductive age may be detected only by comparing children with asymmetrical and symmetrical growth in utero, or children with LBW and catch-up growth with children without catch-up growth.

It is also possible that some other factors, not controlled in this study, influencing a woman’s physiology during development (especially during puberty), might impact the hypothesized relationship between birth parameters and morphological femininity. Such interaction among characteristics resulting from foetal programming (e.g. adult estradiol level) and from adult lifestyle, may be important in determining the relationship between size at birth and adult morphological femininity. For instance, in some countries, low birth weight is not related to an increased risk of atherosclerosis [69] or breast cancer [38], probably due to the lifestyle factors, such as high physical activity, diet and low body weight. A large, population-based study from Denmark has shown no effect of age at menarche on breast cancer risk after adjustment for childhood growth patterns [2]. Furthermore, childhood lifestyle has been shown to affect the prepubertal or pubertal sex hormones levels as physical activity is negatively related with estradiol levels in young girls [77]. On the other hand, Jasieńska et al. [33] showed that birth size was negatively related with a woman’s vulnerability to ovarian suppression in response to her life-style factors, thus possible differences in life-style among our participant during their ontogenetic development, should not obscure the hypothesized relationship between birth parameters and adult femininity.

The results of the study did not confirm our hypothesis on the positive correlation between birth parameters, indicating conditions during prenatal development that are related with life-time exposure to estradiol level, and facial and body femininity in women of reproductive age. As such, our study adds to a growing literature that questions current suggestions about the signal content of morphologically feminine traits.

Methods

This research was a part of a broader project on women’s health, that included 209 participants (Mage = 28.51, SD = 2.37), recruited via social networks and information in the local media. Present study is one in a series, examining the relationship between women’s appearance, facial and body morphology and a wide range of measures of a woman’s biological condition. Data collected in the main project included information on hormone levels, oxidative stress and inflammation markers, blood biochemical markers, morphological measurements, and birth parameters. The protocol used to recruit participants and collect data was approved by the local ethical committee. For each participant, the general purpose of the study was explained and the written consent was obtained for participation in the study and use of data for scientific purposes.

Study sample & procedure

Women were selected for participation if they met the following criteria: age between 25 and 34 years, regular menstrual cycles and no fertility problems (including polycystic ovary syndrome and endometriosis), nulliparous, no current or chronic diseases, no hormonal contraception and no medication for at least 6 months before the study. Furthermore, only women born at term (37–41), with no evidence for a syndromic, chromosomal, or infectious aetiology of low birthweight were included. Participants were invited to participate in the study between the second and the fourth day of menstrual cycle (early follicular phase).

Facial photographs were taken and body measurements were conducted. Women completed the study survey, containing questions on date of birth, past and current health problems, including hormonal disorders. Biochemical blood tests were also performed in order to verify general health status, and fasting blood sample was collected for hormonal analyses. Data on birth weight and length were obtained from the participants’ personal “health books”, containing records about birth size, health condition at birth, and any parturition problems. Ponderal index at birth was calculated as birth weight/(birth length)3 and expressed in kg/m3. Ponderal index may be a better indicator of a newborn nutritional status than birth weight, in the same way that BMI (rather than adult body weight) better reflects adult nutritional status.

Based on collected information and health measurements 44 participants were excluded from the analyses due to the following reasons: 1) other than early follicular phase of menstrual cycle (N = 5); 2) chronic health problems (N = 11); 3) being born as a twin (N = 2); 4) preterm births (N = 6); 5) incomplete data on birth parameters (N = 19); 6) testosterone level exceeding 3SD of the mean testosterone level (N = 1). The final sample included 165 women of mean age 28.47 years (SD = 2.39).

Femininity measurements

Facial femininity was estimated based on facial width-to-height ratio (fWHR) and facial shape sexual dimorphism (facial SD) measured in the photos. Face images were taken under standardized photographic conditions with digital still camera (Nikon D7100 with Tamron SP AF 17-50 mm F/2.8 XR Di II LD IF camera lens). Camera-to-head distance and camera settings were held constant. Participants had no make-up, were asked to have a neutral facial expression, to remove glasses or earrings, and to wear a hairband.

fWHR was measured as the distance between the left and right boundary of the face (width) divided by the distance between the upper lip and the highest point of the eye-lid (height), following Stirrat & Perrett [72], using WebMorph. Lower fWHR values indicate more feminine face. Face-shape sexual dimorphism was measured from each photograph, using a vector analysis method [25], following methodology from Cai et al. [6], using code for R script by Holzleitner et al. (https://osf.io/98qf4/; R script for analysing sexual dimorphism scores following Scott et al. [68] and Komori et al. [41]). Lower score indicates more feminine face shape. An additional adult 50 male (Mage = 27.67 years, SD = 3.14 years) and 50 female (Mage = 25.92 years, SD = 1.85 years) faces (recruited from the same population) were used to build the model used to calculate sexual dimorphism scores.

Body femininity was estimated based on waist-to-hip ratio (WHR) and breast size. Circumferences of waist, hips, breast and under breast were measured to the nearest millimetre. WHR was assessed by the circumference of the waist divided by the circumference of the hips. WHR is inversely related with femininity. Breast size was assessed by the ratio of circumference of the breast to the under-breast circumference. Breast volume is positively related with femininity. Height and weight were measured and BMI was calculated. All measurements (waist, hips, breast and under breast circumference, weight and height) were taken twice and means of the two measurements were used in the analyses.

Hormones levels analyses

In order to evaluate serum testosterone and estradiol levels fasting blood samples were collected into EDTA Vacutainers (BD®). Serum was separated by centrifugation then portioned into micro-tubes and stored at -80 °C until analyses.

Serum total testosterone (tT) concentration was assayed using ELISA commercial kits (Demeditec Diagnostics GmbH® cat. no. DE1559). Samples preparation and assay procedure were performed in accordance with the manual supplied with the kit. Samples, standards (included in each assay) were measured in duplicate. Optical density (absorbance) was measured using microplate reader (ASYS UVM, Biochrom®) set to 450 nm. Testosterone concentration was calculated from the standard curve and expressed in ng/ml. Inter- and intra-assay coefficient of variability (CV) were less than 10% and less than 4.2% respectively, indicating good test precision and repeatability.

Estradiol level (E2) was measured on Roche Cobas analyser in a certified analytical laboratory (DIAGNOSTYKA®), using Roche’s technology (ElectroChemiLuminescence methods). The serum estradiol concentration was expressed in pg/ml.

Statistical methods

The values of ponderal index, fWHR, WHR, breast size, BMI, E2 and tT levels were log-transformed for normalisation purposes, due to their strongly skewed or leptokurtic distribution.

First, to explore the data we run zero-order correlation analyses between birth parameters and femininity measurements. Additionally, we ran zero-order correlation analyses between various measures of morphological femininity and between the measures of femininity and sex steroids levels. Subsequently, we verified if the relationship between birth parameters and femininity can be detected when controlled for the potential confounders that might impact femininity (body adiposity and adult sex hormones levels) or birth weight (pregnancy week at birth). Thus, we ran four multiple regression analyses with the measurements of femininity as the dependent variables and birth weight, pregnancy week at birth, E2, testosterone, and BMI as predictors. Another four regression analyses were run for the femininity measurements as dependent variables and birth ponderal index, pregnancy week at birth, E2, testosterone and BMI as predictors.

In order to verify possible non-linear relationship between birth parameters and adult morphological femininity, we have also divided participants into three groups, based on terciles of birth weight, and compared the values of femininity measurements between women from the first (< 3200 g) and the third tercile (> 3555 g), using t-test. We have also conducted similar analysis for birth ponderal index, comparing women from the first (< 19.97 kg/m3) and the third tercile (> 22.16 kg/m3) of birth ponderal index, in terms of morphological femininity, using t-test.

Analyses were performed with Statistica 12.0 software. The results were interpreted as statistically significant if p < 0.05.

Availability of data and materials

Data available at https://osf.io/625bh/?view_only=1779b50ee0fe41428dbd45b1e6ee5706

Abbreviations

E2 :

estradiol level

tT:

total testosterone level

fWHR:

facial width-to-height ratio

WHR:

waist-to-hip ratio

BMI:

Body Mass Index

Facial SD:

facial shape sexual dimorphism

SD:

standard deviation

References

  1. Adair LS. Size at birth predicts age at menarche. Pediatrics. 2001;107(4):e59.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  2. Ahlgren M, Melbye M, Wohlfahrt J, Sørensen TI. Growth patterns and the risk of breast cancer in women. N Engl J Med. 2004;351(16):1619–26.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  3. Barker DJ. The long-term outcome of retarded fetal growth. Clin Obstet Gynecol. 1997;40(4):853–63.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  4. Bird BM, Jofré VSC, Geniole SN, Welker KM, Zilioli S, Maestripieri D, et al. Does the facial width-to-height ratio map onto variability in men's testosterone concentrations? Evol Hum Behav. 2016;37(5):392–8.

    Google Scholar 

  5. Booth EA, Marchesi M, Kilbourne EJ, Lucchesi BR. 17β-estradiol as a receptor-mediated cardioprotective agent. J Pharmacol Exp Ther. 2003;307(1):395–401.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  6. Cai Z, Hahn AC, Zhang W, Holzleitner IJ, Lee AJ, DeBruine LM, Jones BC. No evidence that facial attractiveness, femininity, averageness, or coloration are cues to susceptibility to infectious illnesses in a university sample of young adult women. Evol Hum Behav. 2019;40(2):156–9.

    Google Scholar 

  7. Chen S, Liu Z, Zhu H, Yang H, Gong F, Wang L, et al. Height at three months can indicate overweight at two years in catch-up growth of small for gestational age infants. Sci Rep. 2018;8(1):1–7.

    Google Scholar 

  8. Cheong JN, Wlodek ME, Moritz KM, Cuffe JS. Programming of maternal and offspring disease: impact of growth restriction, fetal sex and transmission across generations. J Physiol. 2016;594(17):4727–40.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  9. Cianfarani S, Germani D, Branca F. Low birthweight and adult insulin resistance: the “catch-up growth” hypothesis. Arch Dis Child Fetal Neonatal Ed. 1999;81(1):F71–3.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  10. Clemente MG, Capobianco G, Galasso PM, Dessole F, Virdis G, Sanna MG, Olzai MG, Argiolas L, Dessole S, Antonucci R. Postnatal Growth in a Cohort of Sardinian Intrauterine Growth-Restricted Infants. BioMed Research Int. 2017;9382083.

  11. Crespi EJ, Denver RJ. Ancient origins of human developmental plasticity. Am J Hum Biol. 2005;17(1):44–54.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Davies MJ, Norman RJ. Programming and reproductive functioning. Trends Endocrinol Metabolism. 2002;13(9):386–92.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  13. De Bruin JP, Dorland M, Bruinse HW, Spliet W, Nikkels PGJ, Te Velde ER. Fetal growth retardation as a cause of impaired ovarian development. Early Hum Dev. 1998;51(1):39–46.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. DeBruine LM. Women’s preferences for male facial features. In: Evolutionary perspectives on human sexual psychology and behavior. New York: Springer; 2014. p. 261–75.

    Google Scholar 

  15. De Stavola BL, Hardy R, Kuh D, dos Santos Silva I, Wadsworth M, Swerdlow AJ. Birthweight, childhood growth and risk of breast cancer in a British cohort. Br J Cancer. 2000;83(7):964–8.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  16. Eisenbruch AB, Lukaszewski AW, Simmons ZL, Arai S, Roney JR. Why the wide face? Androgen receptor gene polymorphism does not predict men’s facial width-to-height ratio. Adapt Hum Behav Physiol. 2018;4(2):138–51.

    Google Scholar 

  17. Fink B, Neave N, Seydel H. Male facial appearance signals physical strength to women. Am J Hum Biol. 2007;19(1):82–7.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Finstad SE, Emaus A, Potischman N, Barrett E, Furberg AS, Ellison PT, et al. Influence of birth weight and adult body composition on 17β-estradiol levels in young women. Cancer Causes Control. 2009;20(2):233–42.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Fowke JH, Longcope C, Hebert JR. Macronutrient intake and estrogen metabolism in healthy postmenopausal women. Breast Cancer Res Treat. 2001;65(1):1–10.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  20. Gluckman PD, Hanson MA. Living with the past: evolution, development, and patterns of disease. Science. 2004;305(5691):1733–6.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  21. Gray AW, Boothroyd LG. Female facial appearance and health. Evol Psychol. 2012;10(1):147470491201000108.

    Google Scholar 

  22. Han C, Hahn AC, Fisher CI, Debruine LM, Jones BC. Women's facial attractiveness is related to their body mass index but not their salivary cortisol. Am J Hum Biol. 2016;28(3):352–5.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Hodges-Simeon CR, Gurven M, Gaulin SJ. The low male voice is a costly signal of phenotypic quality among Bolivian adolescents. Evol Hum Behav. 2015;36(4):294–302.

    Google Scholar 

  24. Hodges-Simeon CR, Hanson Sobraske KN, Samore T, Gurven M, Gaulin SJ. Facial width-to-height ratio (fWHR) is not associated with adolescent testosterone levels. PLoS One. 2016;11(4):e0153083.

  25. Holzleitner IJ, Hunter DW, Tiddeman BP, Seck A, Re DE, Perrett DI. Men's facial masculinity: when (body) size matters. Perception. 2014;43(11):1191–202.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Hübinette A, Lichtenstein P, Ekbom A, Cnattingius S. Birth characteristics and breast cancer risk: a study among like-sexed twins. Int J Cancer. 2001;91(2):248–51.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Ibáñez L, Potau N, Enriquez G, De Zegher F. Reduced uterine and ovarian size in adolescent girls born small for gestational age. Pediatr Res. 2000;47(5):575.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Ibáñez L, Potau N, Francois I, de Zegher F. Precocious pubarche, hyperinsulinism, and ovarian hyperandrogenism in girls: relation to reduced fetal growth. J Clin Endocrinol Metabolism. 1998;83(10):3558–62.

    Google Scholar 

  29. Israel H. Pubertal influence upon the growth and sexual differentiation of the human mandible. Arch Oral Biol. 1969;14(6):583–90.

    Google Scholar 

  30. Jasieńska G, Ellison PT. Physical work causes suppression of ovarian function in women. Proc Royal Soc London Series B. 1998;265(1408):1847–51.

    Google Scholar 

  31. Jasieńska G, Ellison PT. Energetic factors and seasonal changes in ovarian function in women from rural Poland. Am J Hum Biol. 2004;16(5):563–80.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Jasieńska G, Thune I, Ellison PT. Fatness at birth predicts adult susceptibility to ovarian suppression: an empirical test of the predictive adaptive response hypothesis. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2006a;103(34):12759–62.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Jasieńska G, Ziomkiewicz A, Lipson SF, Thune I, Ellison PT. High ponderal index at birth predicts high estradiol levels in adult women. Am J Hum Biol. 2006b;18(1):133–40.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Jasieńska G, Ziomkiewicz A, Ellison PT, Lipson SF, Thune I. Large breasts and narrow waists indicate high reproductive potential in women. Proc Royal Soc London Series B: Biological Sciences. 2004;271(1545):1213–7.

    Google Scholar 

  35. Johnston VS. Female facial beauty: the fertility hypothesis. Pragmatics Cognition. 2000;8(1):107–22.

    Google Scholar 

  36. Jones AL. The influence of shape and colour cue classes on facial health perception. Evol Hum Behav. 2018;39(1):19–29.

    Google Scholar 

  37. Kapiszewska M, Miskiewicz M, Ellison PT, Thune I, Jasienska G. High tea consumption diminishes salivary 17β-estradiol concentration in polish women. Br J Nutr. 2006;95(5):989–95.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  38. Kar SP, Andrulis IL, Brenner H, Burgess S, Chang-Claude J, Considine D, et al. The association between weight at birth and breast cancer risk revisited using Mendelian randomisation. Eur J Epidemiol. 2019;34(6):591–600.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  39. Kelishadi R, Haghdoost AA, Jamshidi F, Aliramezany M, Moosazadeh M. Low birthweight or rapid catch-up growth: which is more associated with cardiovascular disease and its risk factors in later life? A systematic review and cryptanalysis. Paediatrics Int Child Health. 2015;35(2):110–23.

    Google Scholar 

  40. Kirchengast S, Gartner M. Changes in fat distribution (WHR) and body weight across the menstrual cycle. Collegium Antropologicum. 2002;26:47.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. Komori M, Kawamura S, Ishihara S. Multiple mechanisms in the perception of face gender: effect of sex-irrelevant features. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. 2011;37(3):626.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Kordsmeyer TL, Freund D, Pita SR, Jünger J, Penke L. Further evidence that facial width-to-height ratio and global facial masculinity are not positively associated with testosterone levels. Adapt Hum Behav Physiol. 2019;5(2):117–30.

    Google Scholar 

  43. Kramer RS, Jones AL, Ward R. A lack of sexual dimorphism in width-to-height ratio in white European faces using 2D photographs, 3D scans, and anthropometry. PloS One. 2012;7(8):e42705.

  44. Kuzawa CW. Fetal origins of developmental plasticity: are fetal cues reliable predictors of future nutritional environments? Am J Hum Biol. 2005;17(1):5–21.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Law Smith MJ, Perrett DI, Jones BC, Cornwell RE, Moore FR, Feinberg DR, et al. Facial appearance is a cue to oestrogen levels in women. Proc R Soc B. 2006;273(1583):135–40.

    Google Scholar 

  46. Lefevre CE, Lewis GJ, Bates TC, Dzhelyova M, Coetzee V, Deary IJ, Perrett DI. No evidence for sexual dimorphism of facial width-to-height ratio in four large adult samples. Evol Hum Behav. 2012;33(6):623–7.

    Google Scholar 

  47. Lipson SF, Ellison PT. Comparison of salivary steroid profiles in naturally occurring conception and non-conception cycles. Hum Reprod. 1996;11(10):2090–6.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  48. Marcinkowska UM, Holzleitner IJ. Stability of women's facial shape throughout the menstrual cycle. Proc R Soc B. 2020;287(1924):20192910.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  49. Marcinkowska UM, Kozlov MV, Cai H, Contreras-Garduño J, Dixson BJ, Oana GA, et al. Cross-cultural variation in men's preference for sexual dimorphism in women's faces. Biol Lett. 2014;10(4):20130850.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  50. Maršál K. Physiological adaptation of the growth-restricted fetus. Best Practice Res Clin Obstetrics Gynaecol. 2018;49:37–52.

    Google Scholar 

  51. Matthews CE, Fowke JH, Dai Q, Bradlow HL, Jin F, Shu XO, et al. Physical activity, body size, and estrogen metabolism in women. Cancer Causes Control. 2004;15(5):473–81.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  52. Mellemkjær L, Olsen ML, Sørensen HT, Thulstrup AM, Olsen J, Olsen JH. Birth weight and risk of early-onset breast cancer (Denmark). Cancer Causes Control. 2003;14(1):61–4.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  53. Milich KM, Deimel C, Schaebs FS, Thornburg J, Deschner T, Vitzthum VJ. Links between breast cancer and birth weight: an empirical test of the hypothesized association between size at birth and premenopausal adult progesterone concentrations. Hormones Cancer. 2015;6(4):182–8.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  54. Mondragón-Ceballos R, García Granados MD, Cerda-Molina AL, Chavira-Ramírez R, Hernández-López LE. Waist-to-hip ratio, but not body mass index, is associated with testosterone and estradiol concentrations in young women. Int J Endocrinol. 2015;654046.

  55. Moore FR, Smith ML, Taylor V, Perrett DI. Sexual dimorphism in the female face is a cue to health and social status but not age. Personal Individ Differ. 2011;50(7):1068–73.

    Google Scholar 

  56. Morrison JL, Duffield JA, Muhlhausler BS, Gentili S, McMillen IC. Fetal growth restriction, catch-up growth and the early origins of insulin resistance and visceral obesity. Pediatr Nephrol. 2010;25(4):669–77.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  57. Opdahl S, Nilsen TIL, Romundstad PR, Vanky E, Carlsen SM, Vatten LJ. Association of size at birth with adolescent hormone levels, body size and age at menarche: relevance for breast cancer risk. Br J Cancer. 2008;99(1):201.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  58. Pedersen AL, Nelson LH, Saldanha CJ. Centrally synthesized estradiol is a potent anti-inflammatory in the injured zebra finch brain. Endocrinology. 2016;157(5):2041–51.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  59. Perrett DI, Lee KJ, Penton-Voak I, Rowland D, Yoshikawa S, Burt DM, et al. Effects of sexual dimorphism on facial attractiveness. Nature. 1998;394(6696):884–7.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  60. Pike MC, Spicer DV, Dahmoush L, Press MF. Estrogens, progestogens, normal breast cell proliferation, and breast cancer risk. Epidemiol Rev. 1993;15(1):17.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  61. Platz, E., & Newman, R. (2008). Diagnosis of IUGR: traditional biometry. In seminars in perinatology (Vol. 32, no. 3, pp. 140-147). WB Saunders.

  62. Puts DA, Bailey DH, Cárdenas RA, Burriss RP, Welling LL, Wheatley JR, Dawood K. Women's attractiveness changes with estradiol and progesterone across the ovulatory cycle. Horm Behav. 2013;63(1):13–9.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  63. Puts DA, Jones BC, DeBruine LM. Sexual selection on human faces and voices. J Sex Research. 2012;49(2–3):227–43.

    Google Scholar 

  64. Resnik R. Intrauterine growth restriction. Obstet Gynecol. 2002;99(3):490–6.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  65. Rhodes G, Chan J, Zebrowitz LA, Simmons LW. Does sexual dimorphism in human faces signal health? Proc Royal Society B. 2003;270(suppl_1):S93–5.

    Google Scholar 

  66. Rhodes G, Hickford C, Jeffery L. Sex-typicality and attractiveness: are supermale and superfemale faces super-attractive? Br J Psychol. 2000;91(1):125–40.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  67. Scott IM, Clark AP, Josephson SC, Boyette AH, Cuthill IC, Fried RL, et al. Human preferences for sexually dimorphic faces may be evolutionarily novel. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2014;111(40):14388–93.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  68. Scott IM, Pound N, Stephen ID, Clark AP, Penton-Voak IS. Does masculinity matter? The contribution of masculine face shape to male attractiveness in humans. PLoS One. 2010;5(10):e13585.

  69. Scrimshaw NS. The relation between fetal malnutrition and chronic disease in later life: good nutrition and lifestyle matter from womb to tomb. BMJ. 1997;315:825.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  70. Sharma S, Kohli C, Johnson L, Bennet L, Brusselaers N, Nilsson PM. Birth size and cancer prognosis: a systematic review and meta-analysis. J Dev Orig Hlth Dis. 2020;11(4):309–316.

  71. Son HJ, Kim N, Song CH, Lee SM, Lee HN, Surh YJ. 17β-estradiol reduces inflammation and modulates antioxidant enzymes in colonic epithelial cells. Korean J Internal Med. 2020;35(2):310.

    Google Scholar 

  72. Stirrat M, Perrett DI. Valid facial cues to cooperation and trust: male facial width and trustworthiness. Psychol Sci. 2010;21(3):349–54.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  73. Takakura M, Kuroda T. Morphologic analysis of dentofacial structure in patients with acromegaly. Int J Adult Orthodontics Orthognathic Surg. 1998;13(4):277–88.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  74. Thornhill R, Gangestad SW. Facial sexual dimorphism, developmental stability, and susceptibility to disease in men and women. Evol Hum Behav. 2006;27(2):131–44.

    Google Scholar 

  75. Tworoger SS, Eliassen AH, Missmer SA, Baer H, Rich-Edwards J, Michels KB, et al. Birthweight and body size throughout life in relation to sex hormones and prolactin concentrations in premenopausal women. Cancer Epidemiol Prevention Biomarkers. 2006;15(12):2494–501.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  76. Van Wyk L, Boers K, van Wassenaer-Leemhuis A, van der Post J, Bremer H, Delemarre F, et al. Post-natal catch-up growth after suspected fetal growth restriction at term. Front Endocrinol. 2019;10:274.

    Google Scholar 

  77. Wada K, Nakamura K, Tamai Y, Tsuji M, Masue T, Ando K, Nagata C. Associations of birth weight and physical activity with sex steroids in preschool Japanese children. Cancer Causes Control. 2012;23(2):231–8.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  78. Walker SE. Estrogen and autoimmune disease. Clin Rev Allergy Immunol. 2011;40(1):60–5.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

We are grateful for Barbara Borkowska and Patrycja Kulik for assistance in data collection.

Funding

This work was supported by National Science Centre, Poland; Grant number 015/19/B/NZ8/02061.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

A.Ż., J.N. and B.P. conceived the original idea. A.Ż. and J.N. carried out the experiment. A.Ż. wrote the manuscript with the help of J.N. and B.P. All authors have read and approved the manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Agnieszka Żelaźniewicz.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

The protocol used to recruit participants and collect data, prepared in accordance with declaration of Helsinki, was approved by Ethics Committee of Lower Silesian Chamber of Physicians (DIL, Wrocław, Poland) (document nr 2/BO/2016). The general purpose of the study was explained and written consent was obtained for participation in the study and use of data for scientific purposes from all participants.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that there is no conflict of interest regarding the publication of this article.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Żelaźniewicz, A., Nowak, J. & Pawłowski, B. Birth size and morphological femininity in adult women. BMC Evol Biol 20, 102 (2020). https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/s12862-020-01670-z

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/s12862-020-01670-z

Keywords