Skip to main content
  • Research article
  • Open access
  • Published:

Bolder guppies do not have more mating partners, yet sire more offspring

Abstract

Background

Intra-individual stable but inter-individually variable behaviours, i.e. personalities, are commonly reported across diverse animal groups, yet the reasons for their maintenance remain controversial. Therefore, studying fitness consequences of personality traits is necessary to discriminate between alternative explanations.

Results

Here, I measured boldness, a highly repeatable personality trait, and reproductive success in male guppies, Poecilia reticulata. I found that bolder males had higher reproductive success than their shyer conspecifics and they sired offspring with females who had larger clutches.

Conclusions

This result provides direct evidence for fitness consequences of boldness in the guppy. It suggests that the effect may be driven by bolder males mating with more fecund females.

Background

In the last few decades, the existence of consistent behaviour patterns that differ among individuals, termed personality traits, has been described in many species [1,2,3]. An early hypothesis regarding the evolution of such traits suggested that they evolve neutrally, by the sole mechanisms of the mutation-drift balance [4]. An alternative explanation was that personality traits could influence an individual’s fitness, and in the last few decades researchers have indeed found some evidence for associations between personality traits and fitness components (reviewed in [5]). Both, studies supporting such associations [6,7,8], as well as not [9,10,11], have been published to date, and thus no consistent pattern of the general effect of personalities on fitness has yet emerged.

Boldness, defined as the willingness to be active in a situation when such behaviour is potentially risky (e.g. [12, 13]), is one of the most studied personality traits across animal species. In the existing literature, some studies have found support for a link between boldness and reproductive success, a pivotal component of individual fitness. In a meta-analysis from 2008, Smith and Blumstein [6] reported a moderately positive effect of boldness on reproductive success, but only in captive animals. Several studies have been published since, of which some have reported a similar relationship, for example, higher reproductive success of bolder individuals in Eastern chipmunks, Tamias striatus [14], and zebrafish, Danio rerio [15, 16], and longer survival of bolder guppies when exposed to a predator [17]. In contrast, another study found a negative association between boldness and female fecundity in mosquitofish (Gambusia affinis) [18], indicating that negative consequences of being bold exist in this species, it is however important to note that lower fecundity may not necessarily translate into reduced reproductive success. Similarly, some studies suggest a negative effect of boldness on survival (reviewed in [6]). In addition to these positive and negative associations a number of studies found no association between boldness and fitness (e.g. [9, 10]). Thus, to date we have no overall consensus regarding the relationship between boldness and fitness. More data are needed to make generalizations about non-neutrality, and direction of the effect.

Here, I used guppies to examine the association between boldness and male reproductive success. The guppy is a small tropical freshwater fish which has served as a model in studies of evolution and sexual selection (e.g. [19,20,21,22]). In the last three decades it has also served increasingly as a model for studying the causes and consequences of personality variation (e.g. [23,24,25,26,27]). Among important determinants of guppy male reproductive success, carotenoid colouration stands out as the ornament most consistently preferred by females across populations (e.g. [21, 23, 28]), and is often correlated to the number of offspring sired [29]. However, Godin and Dugatkin [23] showed that females who previously found colourful males more attractive, switched to choosing males based primarily on their boldness when given the opportunity to assess both traits of potential partners simultaneously. Male boldness was manipulated by placing each male in a transparent tube at different distances from a predator model, so that females were presented with a full range of possible combinations of colour and perceived boldness of their potential partners. Males perceived as bolder (i.e. closer to a predator) were consistently preferred by the experimental females, independent of their carotenoid colouration.

Here, I tested if boldness is associated with reproductive success for guppy males. Eighty randomly chosen males were scored for boldness in an emergence test, and then allowed to mate with groups of females. Male reproductive success, measured as the number of offspring he sired with all mating partners, assessed with parentage analysis based on a set of microsatellite markers, was then correlated with his boldness level. Additionally, to account for the confounding effect of ornamental traits and to test if they are associated to personality in my study population, I measured male carotenoid colouration, i.e. the relative area of their carotenoid spots.

Results

Of the 70 males for which complete data on boldness and colouration was collected, 30 (43%) successfully sired offspring, and the mean number of offspring sired by a father was 5.9. The distribution of boldness scores across aquaria is represented in Additional file 1: Figure S1. The clutches of 18 females were sired by a single male, 16 females had clutches which paternity was assigned to two males, and two females had clutches assigned to three males.

Boldness, orange area and body size did not predict success or failure of a male to reproduce (boldness: z7,69 = − 0.82, p = 0.41; orange area: z7,69 = 0.76, p = 0.45; body size: z7,69 = − 0.01, p = 0.99). There was a significant effect of boldness on the number of offspring sired: bolder males sired more offspring (z7,71 = − 0.38, p = 0.011; Fig. 1). Colouration and body size did not affect male reproductive success (Table 1; Additional file 2: Figure S2, Additional file 3: Figure S3, respectively). Apart from boldness, the number of a males’ mating partners had a strong effect on male reproductive success (z7,71 = 0.89, p < 0.000). However, boldness did not significantly explain male number of mates (z7,71 = − 1.33, p = 0.183; Additional file 4: Figure S4, full model in Table 2), which implies that the positive effect of boldness on male reproductive success was not mediated by bolder males mating with a larger number of females.

Fig. 1
figure 1

The relationship between a males’ boldness, measured as latency (in seconds) to emerge from the shelter, and his reproductive success, measured as the number of offspring sired

Table 1 Predictors of male reproductive success, i.e. number of offspring sired. Generalized linear mixed model with a zero-inflated distribution of model residuals was used. Significant p values are denoted with asterisk
Table 2 Predictors of the number of male mating partners, i.e. females who gave birth to those males’ offspring. Generalized linear mixed model with a zero-inflated distribution of model residuals was used

To further explore the mechanism of potential effect of boldness on reproductive success, I tested if bolder males were mated to females with significantly larger clutches. I found a positive association of a males' boldness with the average clutch size across all females he sired offspring with (z7,71 = − 2.84, p = 0.005; Fig. 2).

Fig. 2
figure 2

The relationship between a males’ boldness, measured as latency (in seconds) to emerge from the shelter, and average clutch size of the females that he mated with

Discussion

Although animal personalities have been extensively studied for over 30 years, we still have limited knowledge about the fitness consequences of personality traits [6, 30]. Here, I looked for associations between boldness, one of the most-studied personality traits in animals, and reproductive success, the crucial component of individual fitness, in a fish species. I found a positive and significant effect of guppy male boldness, measured by their propensity to start exploring a new and possibly dangerous environment, on their reproductive success, measured as the number of offspring sired. This result is consistent with a study on zebrafish, which reported a positive association between male boldness and the number of offspring sired [15].

The higher reproductive success of bold guppy males found in the present study could result from female choice, as earlier reported by Godin and Dugatkin [23], who demonstrated female preference for bolder males. However, this study did not provide direct evidence for the importance of boldness for guppy male fitness, as the authors did not allow for matings. Here, I show that bolder males indeed have higher reproductive success than their shyer male conspecifics. Although the number of mating partners also had a strong effect on male reproductive success, as could be expected if bold males were preferred as mating partners [23], boldness was however not associated with the number of females a male successfully sired offspring with. In line with this result, I also found no effect of boldness on the probability of success or failure to reproduce. This suggests, that some other factor/s rather than increased mating success resulting from sexual attractiveness, is responsible for the effect of boldness on reproductive success.

One possibility could be that the sperm of bold males outcompetes that of shy males. Indeed, Gasparini et al. [31] reported an association between boldness and the number of sperm in the guppy, while Evans et al. [32] showed that intrinsic sperm quality of guppy males plays a crucial role in sperm competition after fertilisation, and the key feature conferring higher success is sperm velocity [33]. Alternatively, the determining factor could be female cryptic post-mating preferences, which have been recently documented in the guppy [34, 35]. However, an intriguing result of this study, i.e. the association between successful male boldness and the average clutch size of his mating partners, suggests another possibility. Since bigger guppy females produce more offspring [36, 37], this result may suggest assortative mating among bolder males and larger females. Indeed, male preference for larger more fecund females has been previously found in this species [38, 39]. Thus, if bolder males are better at gaining access to the most preferred females (either through competition or choice), they are expected to successfully mate with large females. Unfortunately, female size was not measured in the current study. Most variation in size between guppy females stems from female age (they grow throughout their lives). Although females used in the current study were of similar age (5 to 9 months), some size variation between females, resulting in clutch size variation, cannot be excluded. A future experiment is needed to check if bolder males mate preferentially with larger females. Alternatively, bolder males may confer some fitness benefits to their offspring, for example, better survival to parturition. A similar effect has been previously reported in zebra fish, where eggs sired by bold males had improved viability [15]. This could be the case here, as I measured male reproductive success as the number of offspring after birth. Thus, “bigger clutch” ascribed to a female may be an effect of better juvenile survival, rather than of bigger clutch produced. Discrimination between the alternatives discussed above will require further research.

Irrespective of underlying mechanism, my data show that male boldness is associated with higher fitness. This highlights the need for an explanation for the presence of variation in personalities. The experiment here was conducted in the absence of predators. Under predation pressure there could potentially be a boldness-related trade-off between individual whole-life reproductive success and survival, since bold fish behave in a more risky way and are thus expected to be under increased risk of predation. Such scenario seems plausible, and indeed, a study by Dugatkin [40] found a negative effect of guppy boldness on survival. Smith and Blumstein [17] however, showed when exposed to a predator there was a positive effect of boldness and exploration on survival in this species. Furthermore, two studies [41, 42] found differences among guppy populations inhabiting high and low predation sites, but counter intuitively they observed higher levels of boldness under stronger predation pressure. Thus, further exploration of the possible trade-offs between different components of fitness as drivers of variation in guppy personality traits is needed.

I found no effect of male orange colouration on the number of offspring fathered. Carotenoid colouration is known to be costly to express and to be an honest indicator of quality in many species [43]. The trait measured in this study was the relative area of carotenoid colouration. Previous studies on guppies, based on the same measure of colouration, have shown higher reproductive success of colourful males [29], cryptic female choice for colourful males [44], and that colourful males produce faster and more viable sperm, which should increase their reproductive success [45]. However, other authors have reported no effect of colouration on guppy sperm competitiveness [46], no preference of females of a related Poecilia species (P.picta) for colourful males [47], and no fecundity benefits for guppy females mating with more colourful males [48], which is in line with a study on zebrafish [16]. This indicates that factors that we still do not understand contribute to the overall effect of colouration on male guppy reproductive success.

Conclusions

The present study provides direct evidence for fitness consequences of boldness in the guppy. Bolder guppy males were found to have higher reproductive success. This effect was not directly driven by higher number of females they sired offspring with. It may be due to bolder males mating more often with females who gave birth to more offspring, or by positive association between male boldness and offspring survival. These possibilities highlight some potential avenues for future research to address. It could also be interesting to investigate the fitness consequences of boldness in the next generation by keeping track of F1 and comparing life history traits (e.g. survival to reproduction, attractiveness or mating success) of offspring sired by bold and shy males.

Methods

Study population

Experimental fish were descendants of wild-caught Trinidadian guppies collected from Tacarigua. Fish in stock and throughout the experiment were kept in stable conditions: temperature 25 ± 1 °C; 12:12 h light/dark regime; and fed twice per day, once with commercial dry flakes and once with nauplii of Artemia sp.

Experimental design

For logistical reasons the experiment was conducted in two blocks. The first block consisted of 20 males and the second comprised 60. Males in each block were randomly caught from the stock population and put into 3 L tanks in a ZebTEC machine (Tecniplast), which allows to keep identical water conditions in all experimental tanks. After spending 3 days in these conditions, their boldness was measured.

For the emergence test, an aquarium (40x20x30cm) filled to a depth of 10 cm of water was used. The aquarium walls were covered from outside with opaque plastic sheets, to avoid fish being distracted. The aquarium contained a dark, plastic box (10x10x10 cm) which served as refuge and was placed close to one of the aquarium walls. At the beginning of the trial a male was put into the refuge box through a hole in the ceiling, which was immediately covered. After 5 min of acclimatisation the door in the front wall of the box was removed, which was done discretely (by pulling the door up by a string at the side of the aquarium) without the experimenter being seen by the fish. Boldness was measured as the time taken by the male to emerge from the refuge box (i.e. when his whole body was visible through the camera suspended above the aquarium). Males who emerged earlier into the open space of the unfamiliar aquarium were considered bolder. A maximum score of 300 s was assigned to those fish that did not come out within 5 min of removing the door. Immediately after the trial, the fish were released back to the home aquarium to avoid familiarization with the test arena.

Repeatability of emergence test is high within the population studied here (0.64, CI 0.60–0.68, see [49] and Table 2 therein). In short, 51 males were tested twice following the procedure described above, with a 1 week interval between the tests. Repeatability was calculated according to Lessells and Boag [50], by dividing the among-individual variance by the sum of the among- and within-individual variances.

After completing the trials, males were randomly and blindly with respect to their boldness score, assigned to one of two (in block 1) or six (in block 2) aquaria (40x30x30 cm), 10 males per aquarium, and allowed to mate for 1 week with mature (5 to 9 months old) virgin females from the stock population, also 10 per aquarium. Such an arrangement, as opposed to forming individual mating pairs, allowed for male-male competition, and gave females the opportunity to compare and mate with a number of different partners. Thus, this approach enabled me to minimise potential differences in mating motivation and investment in reproduction for both sexes. After 1 week of mating, males were removed, the tips of their tail fins were taken for DNA analysis, and they were photographed on their left side under anaesthesia (MS-222). Females were kept in breeding chambers until parturition, after which their tail-fin tips were also taken. Tail-fin tips were sampled from all male and female F1 guppies. All fin samples were stored in 95% ethanol until DNA extraction. All tests and measurements were carried out blindly with respect to the results of the other analyses.

Body area (excluding tail fin) and the area of carotenoid (orange, red, and yellow) spots of all males from the parental generation were measured from photographs using Image J software [51]. The relative area of carotenoid spots was measured as the sum of the area of all spots divided by the body area.

Molecular analyses

DNA was extracted from tail-fin samples using the MagJet Genomic DNA Kit (Thermo Fisher Scientific) according to the manufacturer’s guidelines. In order to assign F1 individuals to their parents, all individuals were screened for variation at six previously described microsatellite loci: Pret-27 [52], G183 [53], TACA033, AG11 [54], G75 [53], and Pret77 [52]. DNA was amplified in two multiplex polymerase chain reactions using PCR Master Mix (Qiagen); one reaction amplified the first three loci while the other amplified the last three. One primer of each primer pair was fluorescently labelled to enable its identification. The 10-uL PCR mixture contained 5 uL of Master Mix, 0.2–0.4 uM of each primer, and 20–100 ng of genomic DNA. The reaction conditions were as follows: a 15-min denaturation step at 95 °C, followed by 36 cycles of 30 s at 94 °C, 1 min at 52 °C, and 1 min at 72 °C, then 10 min of final extension at 72 °C. PCR products were mixed with a GeneScan LIZ500 size standard and electrophoresed on an ABI 3130xl Genetic Analyser. Genotyping was performed using the ABI software GeneMapper 4.0.

Statistical analysis

Parentage was assigned using COLONY 2.0 [55]. Each of the eight groups of 10 males, 10 females, and their offspring was analysed separately, using the full-likelihood method. In each case, one long run was performed, with the following parameters: high likelihood precision, polygamy allowed for both sexes, and no sibship prior. A father or mother were considered parents of an individual if the associated probability of assignment of the putative offspring was above 0.8 (in 97% of cases this value was above 0.9). The number of offspring assigned as sired by a male was the measure of male reproductive success.

A generalised linear mixed model (GLMM) with a binomial distribution of model residuals was used to test for the effect of personality on the probability that a male reproduces. Another GLMM, with a zero-inflated distribution of model residuals, was used to test for the effect of personality on the reproductive success. All males from parental generation were included in those analyses. Both models included boldness, male orange area, male body size, and aquarium (random factor). In the second model, also the number of mating partners was incorporated as fixed factor. Throughout the paper, ‘mating partner’ is used to refer to any female which a male successfully sired offspring with. Block was not entered in the analyses, as its associated variance was accounted for by a random factor aquarium.

The effect of boldness on the number of partners was analysed with a separate GLMM with a zero-inflated distribution of model residuals. In this model also male orange area and body size were included as covariates, and aquarium as random factor.

To explore if a males' boldness is associated with clutch size of his mating partners, I run a GLMM with a zero-inflated distribution of model residuals with average number of young produced by females mated with a given male as response variable and boldness as a predictor. All continuous variables in all analyses were z-scaled. All tests were performed in R v. 3.6.0 [56], in the packages lme4 [57] and glmmTMB [58].

Availability of data and materials

The raw data generated during this study are included in Additional file 5.

Abbreviations

GLMM:

Generalised linear mixed model

MS-222:

Tricaine methanesulfonate

References

  1. Carere C, Maestripieri D. Animal personalities: behavior, physiology, and evolution. Chicago: University of Chicago Press; 2013.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  2. Dingemanse NJ, Reale D. Natural selection and animal personality. Behaviour. 2005;142:1159–84.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  3. Gosling SD. From mice to men: what can we learn about personality from animal research? Psychol Bull. 2001;127(1):45–86.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Tooby J, Cosmides L. On the universality of human-nature and the uniqueness of the individual - the role of genetics and adaptation. J Pers. 1990;58(1):17–67.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Penke L, Denissen JJA, Miller GF. The evolutionary genetics of personality. Eur J Personal. 2007;21(5):549–87.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. Smith BR, Blumstein DT. Fitness consequences of personality: a meta-analysis. Behav Ecol. 2008;19(2):448–55.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Alvergne A, Jokela M, Lummaa V. Personality and reproductive success in a high-fertility human population. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2010;107(26):11745–50.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  8. Germano J, Nafus M, Perry JC, Hall D, Swaisgood R. Predicting translocation outcomes with personality for desert tortoises. Behav Ecol. 2017;00(00):1–10.

    Google Scholar 

  9. Strong JS, Weladji RB, Holand O, Roed KH, Nieminen M. Personality and fitness consequences of flight initiation distance and mating behavior in subdominant male reindeer (Rangifer tarandus). Ethology. 2017;123(6–7):484–92.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. Brent LJN, Semple S, MacLarnon A, Ruiz-Lambides A, Gonzalez-Martinez J, Platt ML. Personality traits in rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta) are heritable but do not predict reproductive output. Int J Primatol. 2014;35(1):188–209.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Larsen M, Johnsson J, Winberg S, Wilson AD, Hammenstig D, Thornqvist P, Midwood J, Aarestrup K, Hoglund E. Effects of emergence time and early social rearing environment on behaviour of Atlantic salmon: consequences for jevenile fitness and smolt migration. PLoS Biol. 2015;10(3):e0119127.

    Google Scholar 

  12. Wilson DS, Coleman K, Clark AB, Biederman L. Shy bold continuum in pumpkinseed sunfish (Lepomis gibbosus) - an ecological study of a psychological trait. J Comp Psychol. 1993;107(3):250–60.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  13. Reale D, Reader SM, Sol D, McDougall PT, Dingemanse NJ. Integrating animal temperament within ecology and evolution. Biol Rev. 2007;82(2):291–318.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Patterson LD, Schulte-Hostedde AI. Behavioural correlates of parasitism and reproductive success in male eastern chipmunks, Tamias striatus. Anim Behav. 2011;81(6):1129–37.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Ariyomo TO, Watt PJ. The effect of variation in boldness and aggressiveness on the reproductive success of zebrafish. Anim Behav. 2012;83(1):41–6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. Vargas R, Mackenzie S, Rey S. ‘Love at first sight’: the effect of personality and colouration patterns in the reproductive success of zebrafish (Danio rerio). PLoS One. 2018;13(9):e0203320.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  17. Smith BR, Blumstein DT. Behavioral types as predictors of survival in Trinidadian guppies (Poecilia reticulata). Behav Ecol. 2010;21(5):919–26.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Wilson ADM, Godin JGJ, Ward AJW. Boldness and reproductive fitness correlates in the eastern mosquitofish, Gambusia holbrooki. Ethology. 2010;116(1):96–104.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  19. Endler JA. Natural selection on color patterns in Poecilia reticulata. Evolution. 1980;34:76–91.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Houde AE. Effect of artificial selection on male colour patterns on mating preference of female guppies. Proc R Soc Lond B. 1994;256:125–30.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  21. Magurran AE. Evolutionary ecology: the Trinidadian guppy. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2005.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  22. Hughes KA, Houde AE, Price AC, Rodd FH. Mating advantage for rare males in wild guppy populations. Nature. 2013;503(7474):108–10.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Godin JGJ, Dugatkin LA. Female mating preference for bold males in the guppy, Poecilia reticulata. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1996;93(19):10262–7.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  24. Kotrschal A, Lievens E, Dahlbom J, Bundsen A, Semenova S, Sundvik M, Maklakov A, Winberg S, Panula P, Kolm N. Artificial selection on relative brain size reveals a positive genetic correlation between brain size and proacive personality in the guppy. Evolution. 2014;68:1139–49.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  25. Burns JG, Price AC, Thomson JD, Hughes KA, Rodd FH. Environmental and genetic effects on exploratory behavior of high- and low-predation guppies (Poecilia reticulata). Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 2016;70(8):1187–96.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. Brown GE, Elvidge CK, Ramnarine I, Chivers DP, Ferrari MCO. Personality and the response to predation risk: effects of information quantity and quality. Anim Cogn. 2014;17(5):1063–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Gasparini C, Speechley EM, Polverino G. The bold and the sperm: positive assosiation between boldness and sperm number in the guppy. R Soc Open Sci. 2019;6:190474.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  28. Sato A, Karino K. Use of digitally modified videos to examine female mate preference for orange spot coloration of males in the guppy, Poecilia reticulata. Ichthyol Res. 2006;53(4):398–405.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  29. Evans JP, Zane L, Francescato S, Pilastro A. Directional postcopulatory sexual selection revealed by artificial insemination. Nature. 2003;421(6921):360–3.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Mittelbach GG, Ballew NG, Kjelvik MK. Fish behavioral types and their ecological consequences. Can J Fish Aquat Sci. 2014;71(6):927–44.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  31. Andersson M. Female choice selects for extreme tail length in a widowbird. Nature. 1982;299(5886):818–20.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  32. Evans JP, Rosengrave P, Gasparini C, Gemmell NJ. Delineating the roles of males and females in sperm competition. Proc R Soc B Biol Sci. 2013;280(1772):20132047.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  33. Boschetto C, Gasparini C, Pilastro A. Sperm number and velocity affect sperm competitionsuccess in the guppy (Poecilia reticulata). Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 2010;65:813–21.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  34. Gasparini C, Congiu L, Pilastro A. Major histocompatibility complex similarity and sexual selection: different does not always mean attractive. Mol Ecol. 2015;24:4286–95.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Gasparini C, Evans JP. Female control over multiple matings increases the opportunity for postcopulatory sexual selection. Proc R Soc B Biol Sci. 2018;285(1888):20181505.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  36. Reznick D, Yang AP. The influence of fluctuating resources on life-history - patterns of allocation and plasticity in female guppies. Ecology. 1993;74(7):2011–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  37. Sato A, Shimoichi A, Karino K. Copulation type affects parturition in the guppy. Zool Sci. 2011;28(2):98–104.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  38. Dosen LD, Montgomerie R. Female size influences mate preferences of male guppies. Ethology. 2004;110(3):245–55.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  39. Herdman EJE, Kelly CD, Godin JGJ. Male mate choice in the guppy (Poecilia reticulata): do males prefer larger females as mates? Ethology. 2004;110(2):97–111.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  40. Dugatkin LA. Tendency to inspect predators predicts mortality risk in the guppy (Poecilia reticulata). Behav Ecol. 1992;3(2):124–7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  41. Elvidge CK, Ramnarine I, Brown GE. Compensatory foraging in Trinidadian guppies: effects of acute and chronic predation threats. Curr Zool. 2014;60(3):323–32.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  42. Fraser DF, Gilliam JF. Feeding under predation hazard - response of the guppy and Hart's rivulus from sites with contrasting predation hazard. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 1987;21(4):203–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  43. Karino K, Shinjo S, Sato A. Algal-searching ability in laboratory experiments reflects orange spot coloration of the male guppy in the wild. Behaviour. 2006;144:103–13.

    Google Scholar 

  44. Pilastro A, Simonato M, Bisazza A, Evans JP. Cryptic female preference for colorful males in guppies. Evolution. 2004;58(3):665–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Locatello L, Rasotto MB, Evans JP, Pilastro A. Colourful male guppies produce faster and more viable sperm. J Evol Biol. 2006;19(5):1595–602.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Evans JP, Rutstein AN. Postcopulatory sexual selection favours intrinsically good sperm competitors. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 2008;62(7):1167–73.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  47. Breden F, Bertrand M. A test for female attraction to male orange coloration in Poecilia picta. Environ Biol Fish. 1999;55(4):449–53.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  48. Pilastro A, Gasparini C, Boschetto C, Evans JP. Colorful male guppies do not provide females with fecundity benefits. Behav Ecol. 2008;19(2):374–81.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  49. Herdegen-Radwan M. Does inbreeding affect personality traits? Ecol Evol. 2019. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1002/ece3.5487 in press.

  50. Lessells CM, Boag PT. Unrepeatable repeatabilities - a common mistake. Auk. 1987;104(1):116–21.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  51. Rasband WS. ImageJ. U. S. National Institutes of Health B, Maryland; 1997-2018. http://imagej.nih.gov/ij/.

  52. Watanabe T, Yoshida M, Nakajima M, Taniguchi N. Isolation and characterization of 43 microsatellite DNA markers for guppy (Poecilia reticulata). Mol Ecol Notes. 2003;3:487–90.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  53. Shen X, Guanpin Y, Meijie L. Development of 51 genomic microsatellite DNA markers of guppy (Poecilia reticulata) and their application in closely related species. Mol Ecol Notes. 2007;7:302–6.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  54. Olendorf R, Reudi B, Hughes KA. Primers for 12 polymorphic microsatellite DNA loci from the guppy (Poecilia reticulata). Mol Ecol Notes. 2004;4:668–71.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  55. Wang J, Santure AW. Parentage and Sibship inference from multilocus genotype data under polygamy. Genetics. 2009;181(4):1579–94.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  56. R Development Core Team. R: a language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing; 2008.

    Google Scholar 

  57. McClelland EE, Damjanovich K, Gardner K, Groesbeck ZJ, Ma MS, Nibley M, Richardson KS, Wilkinson M, Morrison LC, Bernhardt P, et al. Infection-dependent phenotypes in MHC-congenic mice are not due to MHC: can we trust congenic animals? BMC Immunol. 2004;5(1):14.

  58. Douglas B, Martin M, Ben B, Steve W. Fitting Linear Mixed-Effects Models Using. J Stat Soft. 2015;67(1).

Download references

Acknowledgements

I thank Rafał Zwolak, Jonathan Parrett, two anonymous reviewers and the editor Geoffrey While for their helpful comments on previous versions of the manuscript, Karl Phillips for advise in statistical analyses, Jacek Radwan for discussions and Jarek Raubic for technical support.

Funding

This work was supported by National Centre for Sciences (project number 2014/15/B/NZ8/00222). The funding body played no role in the design of the study and collection, analysis, and interpretation of data and in writing the manuscript.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

The author read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Magdalena Herdegen-Radwan.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

The experimental protocols were approved by the Local Ethics Committee in Poznań (decision number 5/2015 from 14.4.2015). The breeding population is licensed and monitored by the local veterinary inspectorate (licence no. PL30646224 of the Local Veterinary Inspectorate in Poznań).

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The author declares that she has no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary information

Additional file 1. Figure S1.

Distribution of male boldness scores across aquaria (1–8) and blocks (A, B). The boxes represent median ± interquartile range, whiskers denote min and max values, outliers are marked with open dots.

Additional file 2: Figure S2.

The relationship between a males’ reproductive success, measured as the number of offspring sired, and his colouration, measured as the relative area of orange spots.

Additional file 3: Figure S3.

The relationship between a males’ reproductive success, measured as the number of offspring sired, and his body size measure, i.e. body area excluding fins.

Additional file 4: Figure S4.

The relationship between a males’ boldness, measured as latency (in seconds) to emerge from the shelter, and the number of females he sired offspring with.

Additional file 5:

Raw data generated during this study, including each male ID, the aquarium and block he was assigned to, boldness score, number of offspring sired, number of mating partners, orange spots area, body size (excluding fins) and mean clutch size among all of his mating partners.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Herdegen-Radwan, M. Bolder guppies do not have more mating partners, yet sire more offspring. BMC Evol Biol 19, 211 (2019). https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/s12862-019-1539-4

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/s12862-019-1539-4

Keywords